‘A knife is a weapon or a tool according to whether you use
it for disembowelling your enemy or for chopping parsley’
Elaine Morgan
“Would innate psychological variation (on average) between the sexes be incompatible with radical feminism? If there was a degree of such innate variation, what would the implications be for radical feminism and gender criticism?”
When faced with this question (or something very like it – I had
the original questioner help me reconstruct the phrasing) last week, I swerved
in what was probably an infuriating way and said that not only is the question
itself not answerable with current knowledge, it is ‘une question mal posée’ –
a question which interrogates premises that are themselves incorrect or
inconsistent.
It is important to understand what the main objection of
radical feminism to the social system of gender is. I have written about it
here, but to summarise: the moral and political problem, for radical feminists,
rests not in men and women having different roles or exhibiting different
tendencies and behaviours, but the hierarchy of value that we have attached to
any perceived differences, before then naturalising those (perceived)
differences and making the value judgements a de facto class system.
I emphasise ‘perceived’ differences, because any
conversation about the average tendencies or capabilities of people rests on
observation of their behaviour. And the main problem with saying anything
definitive about gendered behaviours is that we don’t have any stable idea which
behaviours are which. In what follows below I attempt to demonstrate by example
that our collective judgements are simply too unreliable to be able to correctly
identify average variations between the sexes, never mind interrogate their origins.
Consider that when girls in childhood play with small anthropomorphic
figurines made from wood, plastic and fabric, we call them dolls. When boys play
with anthropomorphic figurines made of the same materials, we call them action
figures. When men use natural materials like marble or metal to create objects
of aesthetic value, we call it art. When women use natural materials like wool
or clay to create objects of aesthetic value, we call it craft. This is true
even in cases where men and women are using the same materials to create the same
types of objects: in traditional Moroccan practice, for example, weaving
carpets by hand is a women’s job but weaving fabric by hand is a men’s job. The
men’s job, because it is performed by men, who anyway control the majority of
financial resources, was more highly valued and more invested in, creating a
discrepancy in availability of materials and complex tools which led to the
development of the brocade industry. Brocades could then be made with silk and
gold thread (because the men making them had more capital to invest), making
them more lucrative and perpetuating the economic discrepancy. The result is
that carpet weaving is considered a crude domestic craft, but brocade weaving
is an elite industry. Both ‘roles’ involve putting threads in rows and then
pulling other threads through.
Nor do we demand consistency of ourselves when we tell
ourselves stories about why certain seemingly-related tasks are associated with
men and bring financial rewards, whereas other tasks are associated with women
and bring no financial benefits and the expectation of more free labour. Consider
the prevailing narrative of our prehistoric ancestors. Most lay people (and a
not-inconsiderable portion of experts, too) have a picture in their heads of a rigidly
segregated savannah on which men hunted and women gathered. Hunting was
dangerous and prestigious. Gathering was easy, opportunistic, and taken for
granted. The fact that at least as much knowledge must be invested in distinguishing
between ripe and unripe, or safe and poisonous plants, or knowing which parts
of the environment they favour in different seasons, is at least as great as
the expertise needed to track an animal to it lair. Or even the fact that
hunting is an uncertain activity and it is likely that gathering supplied the
majority of critical calories to the group and staved off starvation if hunting
failed. That’s low hanging fruit. Here are two much greater paradoxes with the
standard ‘man
the hunter’ narrative:
When we think of prehistoric hunting, we tend to think of
stuff like mammoths and bison – big game, basically. But the majority of
hunting done by hunter-gatherer groups is not running pell-mell after giraffes;
it is trapping (rabbits, monkeys etc.), netting birds or raiding their nests
for eggs, and fishing. That’s true of even modern hunters, who only do it for
fun. Many more people fish or rabbit course than ride to the hunt. This type of
hunting doesn’t require big time investments, stamina or going too far afield.
Certainly your average rabbit is not that much more exotic a foodstuff than
your average apple. And guess what, it turns out that women did
a lot of this kind of hunting, as you would expect – as well
as participating in the stereotypical big game kind. You can certainly
check a couple of lobster traps with a baby on your back, or whatever limiting
function w imagine held women back from hunting (evolutionary vegetarianism,
perhaps?). Well, guess what again: when activities such as catching small rodents
were recorded among hunter gatherer groups, they were called… Gathering. Because
women gather, men hunt. So if a woman is doing it, it’s got to be gathering. I
haven’t heard of anyone calling climbing for coconuts ‘hunting’ just yet, but
what do you want to bet that someone somewhere has written about it as a more
complex, more intrepid, altogether more Manly activity than mere collection of yams?
[Two examples of the above classification practice can be
found in this
one paper about the Bakola of the Congo region: while gathering is ‘reserved
mostly for women and children’, ‘the important tools regularly used for
gathering include machete, which functions at the same time as a weapon for
killing animals’ – but no mention is made of these animals being ‘hunted’
(earlier in the paper the author also talks about reptile meet being desirable
but mostly killed by happenstance upon bush clearing, another activity
undertaken by the ‘gathering’ women). Honey collection is classified as ‘gathering’
in the paper, but when describing the honey gatherer the author uses the pronoun
‘him’, without explaining what observations led to this seeming discrepancy
with other gathering activities. Other examples abound in the literature.]
The second paradox has to do with agriculture. The standard
picture or the development of agriculture is that it was a technological
advance made by men. Certainly by the time the dust settled on the Agricultural
Revolution (if it can be said to have settled yet, which is by no means
certain), men in the majority of known cultures controlled not only the surplus
produce of farming, but the rights to declare ‘ownership’ of land and of the
people who work the land. Further: at some point before or during this process,
men have arrogated to themselves the right not just to farm vegetables, but to
farm people. By controlling and trading the reproductive potential of women,
men guaranteed not only the food supply, but the labour supply too.
Here’s a question nobody seems to ask: how the hell did they
do it? How on earth did men go from running after bison to knowing which
grasses had kernels that were good to eat and which were, well, grass? How did
they know which berries didn’t kill you and where in the forest they grew? How did
they know what the ground looked like above a promising bit of tuber, and how
deep to dig with their digging sticks? Did women write some sort of Neolithic
Encyclopaedia of Gathering and, I dunno, gift it to men?
The fact is that if we accept the man-the-hunter,
woman-the-gatherer narrative, we’re pretty tied up with the idea that the first
cultivators of crops – even in a small way, by clearing a few weeds around a
promising patch of vegetation – were women. That agriculture was invented by
women. Agriculture is possibly the greatest human revolution of all time, a way
bigger deal than writing or putting people in silly suits on the moon.
Agriculture changed the entire world, rearranged species, transformed or
destroyed or created whole ecological niches. And if we believe that women were
doing the less prestigious activity in the Palaeolithic, we have a pretty big explanatory
gap to fill with regard to how come they suddenly lost their interest in plants
by the Neolithic. It’s the carpet/brocade thing at wok again: whichever is the
more valorised and lucrative activity, it is instinctively ascribed to men.
These two paradoxes are central to a wide complex of stories
we tell ourselves about the past (others are connected to things like pottery:
if women were in charge of cooking, they are likely to have been the nes to invent pottery, and therefore to have been the first to use kilns. Then why do we think the technical improvements to
kilns that set the stage for ore smelting were made by men?). The doll/action
figure duality is a problem with the stories we tell ourselves about the
present. But the inconsistencies in how we look at nature don’t just affect how
we look at human behaviour. We even gender basic inanimate processes.
Everyone knows that sperm are mobile and eggs are motile,
and that sperm race towards the egg and all compete to be the ones who
fertilise it, right? Well now. What if I told you that the egg, sensible
creature that she is, actually sits there serenely and uses a complex
biochemical mechanism to first attract, then identify the most viable sperm,
and once identified, actively seizes and envelops it? The ‘choosy
egg’ hypothesis is slowly beginning to gain ground among experts, and the
fact that it’s only happening slowly, on top of the fact that nobody has ever
thought to actually check what the egg does, from the beginning of
embryological science until now, tell a woeful story about not only our ability
to interpret what we see, but our willingness to even look.
I am a rationalist and a materialist; I believe in evolution,
in biology, in systematic archaeology and in careful evidence gathering as a
means of arriving at, if not The Truth, then at least a fair enough
approximation of a correct interpretation of reality to enable us to successfully
operate in the world and continue to improve people’s material circumstances. As
such the stories above are not a rejection of archaeology, or biology, or even
the toy industry (although the latter can go to its room and think about what
it’s done with regard to the Pink Apartheid of girls). But when people
challenge me to definitively say which parts of our behaviour are Nature and
which are Nurture, how much of sex difference in behaviour is capital-E Evolution and how
much is capital-S Socialisation, I always want to take them back to the
beginning and say: define 'behaviour'.
It’s only once we understand that we interpret
data through stories, and that those stories can be distorting, contradictory,
or absurd, that we can really start the work of picking through the evidence to
decide whether there even are significant behavioural changes between the
sexes. And it’s only once significant behavioural changes consistently isolated and defined that we can start asking ourselves, and developing the complex
methodologies for answering, the question ‘how much of sex difference is
genetic?’ Until then, we’re dealing with mythology, not fact. Mythology, let’s
be clear, is much more powerful than fact. That is why my personal efforts are
directed at busting the myths than ‘proving’ any new facts about women’s brains
or maths skills. And that’s why I give people annoying and seemingly evasive answers
to their reasonable-sounding challenges about sex difference.
Excellent essay. The real question, as you say, is not "Are there innate differences?"
ReplyDeleteIt's why are differences -- however they originate -- assigned such very different value. And how in every instance that assignment has nothing to do with the actual value of the activity to human beings.
Every difference in the world could be 100% genetically programmed. It still wouldn't explain why, for instance, the contribution of sperm results in a whole legal superstructure conferring lots of rights, whereas motherhood is just something women do all the time that, if anything, causes problems for men.
"Further: at some point before or during this process, men have arrogated to themselves the right not just to farm vegetables, but to farm people. By controlling and trading the reproductive potential of women, men guaranteed not only the food supply, but the labour supply too."
ReplyDeleteAbsolute rubbish.
Men invented farming by accident when the mammoth they were chasing tripped over and ploughed a perfect furrow in a field with its tusks. It then skidded back the other way a few times.
ReplyDelete